News / Science News |
How Our Memory Works
NIH, US | JANUARY 25, 2010
Our memory reflects our last retrieval of it rather than an exact account of the original event. When long-term memories are recalled, they become fragile and changeable. New proteins must be produced to reconsolidate retrieved memories and return them to long-term storage in the brain. Recalling a memory, then, opens a window of opportunity for that memory to be changed — what researchers call a "reconsolidation window."
Earlier animal studies showed that certain drugs can block reconsolidation, but using such drugs in people can be problematic.
In 2009, a team led by Dr. Joseph LeDoux of New York University (NYU) developed a way to erase a fear memory in rats without using drugs. The researchers first conditioned rats to fear a tone by pairing it with shocks.
The animals then underwent a process to erase the fear, called extinction training, in which the tone was repeatedly presented without shocks. Both an initial re-exposure to the shock and the subsequent timing of extinction training proved critical.
Fear of the stimulus was erased only in rats trained within a 6-hour reconsolidation window after re-exposure to the feared tone. Fear responses returned in animals if the training began more than 6 hours after re-exposure, when the memory had apparently already solidified.
In a second study, a team led by Dr. Elizabeth Phelps (NYU) set out to see if extinction training could similarly erase a fear in people.
The researchers conditioned human participants to fear colored squares by pairing them with mild wrist shocks.
A day later, the memory was first reactivated by re-exposing participants to the feared squares. Measures of nervous system arousal confirmed that the participants experienced a fear response. Extinction training — repeated exposures to the colored squares without shocks — followed.
A day later, the fear response was banished only in human participants who underwent extinction training soon after fear reactivation. Those whose training began more than 6 hours later remained afraid of the squares. So did a control group that received extinction training without first experiencing reactivation of the fear memory.
To gauge the long-term effects of the training, participants were tested a year later. They were given 4 unsignalled shocks to re-instate their fear and then shown the colored squares. Only those who had undergone extinction training within the reconsolidation window were largely spared a fear response to the squares.
"This adaptive update mechanism appears to have evolved to allow new information available at the time of retrieval to be incorporated into the brain's original representation of the memory," explains Phelps. "Our memory reflects our last retrieval of it rather than an exact account of the original event."
Scientists have for the first time selectively blocked a conditioned fear memory in humans by using a behavioral technique. The advance represents a safe, easily implemented way to prevent the return of a fearful memory.